London, British Library MS Harley 1775 (Harley Gospels), fol. 373v. John 1.14 per cola et commata, in scriptio continua. |
London, British Library MS Additional 89000 (St Cuthbert Gospel), fol. 1v. John 1.14 with canonical word separation. |
More controversial, however, is the argument that Saenger builds on top of this history, an argument encapsulated in the subtitle of the book: The Origins of Silent Reading. Briefly, Saenger claims that word separation was, in the Middle Ages, ‘the crucial element in the change to silent reading’. Silent reading, in turn, facilitated ‘reference reading’ – the technique of scanning texts quickly to find specific items of information – and contributed to the shift from the idea of reading as a public activity, as it was in the ancient world, to the idea of reading as a private activity, as it is in the modern world. I am, of course, greatly oversimplifying Saenger’s ideas here, but if you want all the details, you should read the book.
Saenger’s thesis may be attractive, not least the argument that a seemingly innocuous and subtle encoding practice – introducing a bit of white space between words in written texts – has had such far-reaching technological and social effects. But is there a straightforward causal relationship between word separation and silent reading? I’m not so sure.
St Ambrose in his study, Spanish, c 1500. Image: Metropolitan Museum of Art. |
There are writing systems today that do not employ word separation: for example, Thai, which uses a syllabic alphabet (abugida). The lack of spaces between words in Thai writing does not make it impossible, or even difficult, to read silently. Saenger assumes that Chinese writing is a simple logographic script in which there are spaces between words because each character represents a word, and credits this circumstance with a greater facility in silent reading among Chinese children and adults; but this view is based on a drastic oversimplification of the way Chinese writing works (more accurate descriptions of Chinese script describe it as semanto-phonetic or logosyllabic) and obscures the fact that Chinese is also written in scriptio continua.
putting spaces between words is not a precondition for silent readingThere is thus no necessary connection between word-separated script and silent reading, any more than there is between scriptio continua and reading aloud. Putting spaces between words is not a precondition for silent reading, since silent reading of scriptio continua was practiced in the past, as it is today. Word separation by space does seem to increase efficiency in reading (Rayner and Pollatsek 2013), but whether that reading is oral or silent is a separate issue.
If there was, therefore, no simple cause-and-effect relationship between word separation and silent reading in the early Middle Ages, questions still remain. What did word separation accomplish? And where did practices of silent reading come from? Answers to the second question are bound to be complicated, and to some extent lie outside the scope of this project. But we might be able to say a bit more about the history of medieval word separation, especially in early medieval England, which, after all, was one of the places where it developed in a form that continues to the present day.
[Left: John 1.14 in British Library MS Cotton Nero D.iv, the Lindisfarne Gospels, fol. 211v. Latin text c 700, in partially word-separated script; interlinear 10th-century Old English gloss in canonically separated script. Image: British Library.]
And even if we must reject word separation as a necessary condition for silent reading, might it still be legitimate to argue that it helped to disseminate the practice of silent reading more widely in the Middle Ages? I am not so sure of that, either, but at least Saenger deserves credit for getting us to look more closely not only at the words in a written text but also at the spaces between them. In a future blog post, perhaps, I’ll comment on recent research into eye movement and word processing in the context of reading, and what its implications might be for understanding how medieval texts were read.
Yin Liu
References and Further Reading
Saenger, Paul. Space Between Words: The Origins of Silent Reading. Stanford: Stanford UP, 1997.
For evidence that silent reading from scriptio continua was practiced in ancient Greece and Rome and in late Antiquity, see
Gavrilov, A. K. ‘Techniques of Reading in Classical Antiquity.’ Classical Quarterly 47 (1997): 56-73.
Svenbro, Jesper. ‘Archaic and Classical Greece: The Invention of Silent Reading.’ A History of Reading in the West. Ed. Guglielmo Cavallo and Roger Chartier. Trans. Lydia G. Cochrane. Amherst: U of Massachusetts P, 1999. 37-63.
Carruthers, Mary. The Book of Memory: A Study of Memory in Medieval Culture. 2nd ed. Cambridge: Cambridge UP, 2008. See pp. 212-216.
Johnson, William A. Readers and Reading Culture in the High Roman Empire: A Study of Elite Communities. Oxford: Oxford UP, 2010. See especially pp. 3-9, which provide an overview of scholarly debates about the question ‘did the ancients read silently or aloud?’. The consensus among classicists today, Johnson notes, is that they did both.
On silent reading in the Benedictine Rule and Isidore of Seville’s Liber sententiarum, see
Parkes, M. B. ‘Rædan, Areccan, Smeagan: How the Anglo-Saxons Read.’ Anglo-Saxon England 26 (1997): 1-22, at 9-10.
For a summary of recent scholarship on the neuropsychology of reading, see
Rayner, Keith, and Alexander Pollatsek. ‘Basic Processes in Reading.’ The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Psychology. Ed. Daniel Reisberg. Oxford: Oxford UP, 2013. 442-461.
No comments :
Post a Comment